Quasi-Experimental Research

From WikiEducator
Jump to: navigation, search



Quasi-experimental Research

A girl playing violin in The Hague.

Quasi‐experiments are studies that use a research method that resembles an experiment, but the variables that distinguish the groups are not directly manipulated. Typically, quasi‐experimental designs are utilized when the variable that the researcher wishes to study cannot be manipulated for ethical or practical reasons.

For example, a researcher may be interested in the effects of a certain characteristic (sex, height, religion, age, or intelligence) on language development. For ethical reasons, a researcher could not sneak into prenatal classes and inject expectant mothers with a hormone cocktail such that they all gave birth to males. Instead, he or she might wait until the babies were born, and the males would be assigned to one group and the females to the other group. Importantly, the researcher does not randomly assign the participants to the groups; in a sense, Nature does it for the researcher. Because the variable of interest in quasi‐experimental designs is not directly manipulated by the researcher, it is not considered a true independent variable. Therefore, to distinguish variables such as sex, height, sexual preference, and age in quasi‐experiments from true independent variables, they are referred to as subject variables. Subject variables are characteristics that participants possess prior to their assignment to groups.

It is important to remember that only true experiments (discussed in Unit 4) can allow the researcher to determine cause and effect. Nonexperimental research approaches, including using quasi-experimental methods, have several potential confounds. For example, in one study, researchers looked at precocious 13 year-old male and female math students and then compared them 4 decades later on various variables such as education, occupation, income, and retrospective, prospective, and ideal time allocation on family and work. One of the salient gender differences was that mathematically gifted men expressed stronger preferences for developing high-impact careers while the women expressed stronger preferences for and devoted more time to advancing family and community (Lubinski, Benbow, & Kell, 2014).

This was a quasi‐experimental design because students were assigned to groups based on whether they were males or females. The two groups, males and females, probably differed in more than their chromosomes, however, and some of these differences could plausibly account for the study’s results. For example, the males may have followed a different path than the females because of cultural role models, discrimination, parental or societal expectations, or differences in opportunity.

Advantages of quasi-experimental research

  • Quasi-experimental research permits the study of variables that would be impractical, unethical, or impossible to manipulate.
  • Quasi-experimental studies are typically easier to set up than true experimental designs, which require random assignment of subjects.
  • Utilizing quasi-experimental designs minimizes threats to external validity as natural environments do not suffer the same problems of artificiality as compared to a well-controlled laboratory setting.
  • Since quasi-experiments are natural experiments, findings in one may be applied to other subjects and settings, allowing for some generalizations to be made about the population.

Limitations of quasi-experimental research

  • The lack of random assignment in quasi-experimental research introduces several potential threats to internal validity (e.g., selection, selection by maturation, etc.)
  • Causation cannot be established because the experimenter does not have total control over extraneous variables.
Icon activity line.svg
Open Textbook Reading Activity

Read the section on "Quasi-Experimental Research" in Chapter 7 of your textbook (Nonexperimental Research) and distinguish between the different types of designed outlined.